Hyperthymestic syndrome

There’s only one person known to have it.

Her symptoms? A preternatural memory.

Give her any date, and she can

recall the day of the week, usually what the weather was like on that day, personal details of her life at that time, and major news events that occurred . . .

[She] remembers trivial details as clearly as major events. Asked what happened on Aug 16, 1977, she knew that Elvis Presley had died, but she also knew that a California tax initiative passed on June 6 of the following year, and a plane crashed in Chicago on May 25 of the next year, and so forth. Some may have had a personal meaning for her, but some did not.

She’s not an idiot savant. She’s a “fully functioning person.”

Now–isn’t this typical!–she’s been kidnapped by scientists and is being held in a lab where they’re preparing to run a series of MRIs . . . ha ha ha, just kidding about the kidnapping part. She’s volunteered to be studied. We guess.

(I wonder if her mother ate a lot of eggs.)

Hyperthymestic syndrome

More on the malleability of perception

A Cornell University study examines the relationship between what we believe about people and our memory of events involving them.

To conduct the study, David Pizarro, assistant professor of psychology at Cornell, and three colleagues

gave 283 college students a story about a man who walked out on a restaurant bill, including what the man ate and drank and the amount of his bill. Half the participants read that the man walked out on the bill because he “was a jerk who liked to steal,” and half read that the man left without paying because he received an emergency phone call.

“One week later the people who were told he was a jerk remembered a higher bill — from 10 to 25 percent more than the bill actually was. Those who were told he had an emergency phone call remembered a slightly lower-than-actual bill,” said Pizarro.

The researchers appear focused on how these distortions may affect civic institutions (e.g. eyewitness testimony during criminal trials); they also characterize the distortions as stemming from observers’ ethical judgments. In Pizarro’s words: “[O]ur study shows that morally blaming a person can distort memory for the severity of his or her crime or misbehavior.”

But it seems to me that something broader is at work, here. It’s not morals, but motivation. When we witness events involving other people, and string those events together as a narrative, the narrative is shaped by our understanding of the players’ motivations.

At some point, we also “shade in” or heighten certain details within our memory of events in order to capture or express our understanding of it more accurately (as opposed to cataloguing what actually happened).

Kinda like writing fiction ;-)

The full study will be published in a forthcoming issue of the Memory and Cognition.

(Incidentally, the Cornell article I’ve quoted here implies that every single person in the study misremembered the bill. Hmmmmm. I suppose that’s possible . . .)